Bel Guinle
Block: 6
To
what extent should we use our own humanity to study human behavior?
Humanity: “humanness; benevolence.” Humanity: “The fact or
condition of being human; human nature.” Out of all definitions of humanity
these two seemed to e the ones that matched the question the best and since I
cannot decide which one is the better one I will attempt to answer the question
using both definitions. However, since the definitions differ, so might the
answers. If we start with the former definition an important assumption is
made: one characteristic of a human is their benevolence. This implies that
since is it a characteristic of humanity and humanness, it is consequently an
essential quality for the distinction between what is human and what is not.
Being able to act kindly, therefore, should – according to that definition – be
a common trait to humans. Thus, the question could be asking, to what extent
should we use our benevolence to study human behavior? The answer to that
question ties in to emotion since empathy and our ability to relate to another
human being is involved. When the question is phrased with this definition of
humanity I think it is asking us to use our mirror neurons. Through research,
neuroscience has found that whenever we see someone else who is sad or upset,
our mirror neurons are triggered and that allows us to feel empathy – to put
ourselves in that person’s shoes. Using empathy to study human behavior would
allow us to see it through a subjective and specific lens: our own. As soon as
we try to understand what someone else’s sadness feels like, we are relating it
to what sadness feels to us because that is the only sadness we know. By
analyzing human behavior through the use of benevolence and empathy, we are
generating story knowledge of our own. To some extent that is good, because we
will be able to reach conclusions that take into consideration people’s
feelings and well-being rather than practicality alone. On the other hand,
generating story knowledge limits us to seeing just the specificity of the
situation and since we can only know our what we’ve experienced we can’t
exactly generate story knowledge for the person being observed, though we can
imagine. Since we are analyzing human behavior it seems fit that humanness
would be a pre-requisite but if we stop to think about it, in biology, to study
koalas you don’t have to be a koala (bad analogy but the point makes sense).
Maybe the exact thing that makes us relate to the subjects of our study is
what’s getting in the way of reaching an appropriate conclusion. If we stop to
think about it, how can we study human behavior when we participate in such
actions? We aren’t outsiders – maybe to specific actions but not to human
behavior as a whole or else we wouldn’t be humans. But even if that is true we
can’t stop being humans so maybe the way out of the trap is using our humanness
to decode ourselves. If we use benevolence to understand human behavior we will
be trying to relate to another human being, reach conclusions about his actions
and – though probably not being able to generalize to other people – we will
generate knowledge. Knowing more about that person specifically could be
relevant; even though it doesn’t give us insights to human behavior as a whole,
it does allow us to understand specificities and differences between people,
which can be very useful. But this is only one way to see the question.
Taking into consideration the other definition of humanity,
which is simply being human, a lot of possibilities are open to answer the
question. But first we have to consider what makes us human? If our DNA is 99%
identical to those of chimps, is it the 1% that makes so much difference or is
there something else that natural sciences can’t explain? For some time we’ve
been distinguished from other animals by our ability to use tools, to make fire
and to bury our dead. The former of the three has been proven to no longer be
exclusive to humans, since chimps (and even otters) use raw tools. While the
two latter ones may be true, it is hard to see how much they actually
contribute to our being human. The problem with these questions however, is
that they generate a search for something special that will set humans apart
from other animals – that will prove our superiority. Mythologies, religions
and other personal beliefs lead us to think we are the dominating race and that
we are the dominating race for a purpose. In our search for the confirmation of
that belief – that we want so much to be true, we find ourselves making
confirmation bias. Suddenly larger brains, funerals and making fire make us
better, set us apart. If we take all of that into consideration when answering
the question then does the question become: to what extent should we use the
abilities exclusive to humans and that set us apart from other animals to study
human behavior? If we had to consider the abilities only exclusive to us, we
would have to exclude empathy and other emotions, reflexes, and basically
several abilities we have developed over time. We, humans, are not the only
ones who have mirror neurons; one of the earlier studies on mirror neurons was
made on monkeys. Anyone who has a dog or cat (maybe even a fish but I wouldn’t
know) can see that they have emotions – you know when your dog is sad, thankful
or happy. Reflexes are also not exclusive to us; when you pretend to throw a
ball your dog will try to run for it but will stop when he sees it in your hand
again. Clearly what makes us humans composes a lot of other animals too so we
can’t exclude that and focus only on what is exclusive to us. So “human nature”
in this blog answer will mean anything and everything that is characteristic to
humans (exclusively or not). Since that doesn’t relate to kindness and
benevolence only, the opposite of those indifference and dislike (or hate but
hate might be too strong) can be used to study human behavior. The benefits of
using indifference is a more objective point of view, if we feel nothing
towards that person we will be able to assess their behavior as an outsider,
not caring about how that will affect him/her. Studying someone with hate might
have the inverse benefits of studying someone with kindness; we will try to
find a way to see that what the person is doing is wrong and though that
doesn’t sound like a benefit if what the person is doing is actually wrong (if
there is such thing) we will catch up on it earlier. In both ways we analyze
and study human behavior with feelings linked to it, confirmation bias will be
more present; we will either be cheering for that person or against him/her and
that could be an issue in several instances – especially when you are trying to
reach a conclusion that you can later extrapolate to other people. However,
other human qualities are important to be able to study human behavior.
Observation, analysis, logic – all of these are needed when studying almost
anything (human behavior being no exception to that). The ability to look at
something and draw conclusions or to know which variables to control and what
to study – all of these characteristics are part of our knowledge, which is
essentially part of our “condition of being human” (also known as humanity).
Since both ways to analyze the word humanity (and how it
relates to the question) have been unpacked, now I can attempt to answer the
question itself. “To what extent should we use our own humanity to study human
behavior?” I could try to unpack the meaning of study and human behavior but
that would take two other entire essays – so let’s make them as premises. Given
that human behavior means behavior performed by humans and that study means an
investigation/analysis of a subject, “To what extent should we use our own
humanity to study human behavior?” I think that when answering the question
with the second definition of humanity the answer (though I am not too fond of
the word) is always. We should always use our observation, analysis, logic and
other skills that seem fit to the situation. It doesn’t mean we should use all
of them every time, but there is no way to not use them. There would be no way
for a human to perform an investigation if he/she can’t observe and draw
conclusions. So I think, that we should always use the “condition of being
human” when studying human behavior. Now looking at it through the benevolence
lens, I think that it might not always be useful to see the best in every
situation, especially when analyzing human behavior. Milgram’s experiment
shows, for example, that even nice, sweet people end up shocking others if an
authority figure tells them to. If he had used his benevolence in that
experiment, maybe he wouldn’t have taken the precautions of making it fake
shocks; if he been kind and trusted that others would be too, many people could
have died from being shocked. Instead, he used actors and didn’t actually
connect wires. Thus, I don’t think benevolence should always be used when
studying human behavior but if it won’t affect results or harm anyone I think
(at least most people) can benefit from kindness.
WC:
1591
This comment has been removed by the author.
ReplyDeleteArrrggghh. I have written two long comments for you, but both times they were dropped by the system. I'm going to test a few more times before I try again.
ReplyDeleteOK, it seems to be working again.
ReplyDeleteBel, you've written a compelling and unusual response I really like the way you answer the question by following two different paths, considering how two different definitions of the word humanity might lead to different implications and different answers. That overall structure is effective. What's missing, though, is an example to illustrate this. The question asks how we should study humans, so seeing that in action is essential. You need a test case for your theory. What would happen if you applied your two different definitions to a real human science study, such as Milgram's obedience study? Digging into this would give you something to evaluate. Or maybe you'd prefer a real life situation? It doesn't matter what you choose; the important thing is to get an extended example in there.